Nd when two or more judges marked the same error, it was recorded in a final transcript. Second, Study 2C analyzed the neologisms, false starts, dysfluencies, and off-topic comments that have been eliminated in the transcripts in Research 1 and MacKay et al. [2]. Neologisms integrated all non-standard pronunciations of a familiar word; dysfluencies have been “um”s and “uh”s; off-topic comments have been irrelevant remarks about the task or the experimenter (e.g., “How’s that suit you”, where that refers to a self-produced response, and you towards the experimenter); and false begins have been sentence-level revisions or modifications (excluding error corrections), where a speaker began with 1 plan or intended output, then shifted to another. For instance, “they feel it’s–they can not do it simply because it really is also hard” was coded as a false begin since the participant started to say they think it really is too really hard but switched to “they can not do it since it really is too hard”.Brain Sci. 2013,Finally, Study 2C determined the frequency of three kinds of repetition: stutters, unmodified word string repetitions, and elaborative repetitions. Following MacKay and MacDonald [71], stutters involved quick repetitions of word-initial speech sounds, syllables, and words, e.g., “s–school” (repetition of a word-initial speech sound). Unmodified word string repetitions involved immediate repetition of a sequence of words devoid of correction, as in “but it was, however it was”. Elaborative repetitions involved repetition of 1 or additional concepts in distinctly various phrases. The repeated words italicized in (44) illustrate a stutter (it, it) and two elaborative repetitions (that bus, the scrawny bus, and drive it off … it drives it off”, MedChemExpress Tenacissoside H exactly where drives elaborates the idea drive). The repeated words italicized in (45) illustrate an unmodified word string repetition (it is crowded … it’s crowded) and two elaborative repetitions (it’s crowded … as well crowded, and to go around the bus … to get around the bus, exactly where get PubMed ID:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21338877 elaborates conceptual go). The repeated words italicized in (46) illustrate an elaborative repetition (this pie is … the pie right here was back right here, where was elaborates is as + past). (44). H.M.: “Melanie tra … on that bus, the scrawny bus and have it drive it off … it, it drives it off.” (repeated words in italics) (45). H.M.: …she wants to go on the bus … and it is crowded … it really is crowded … Too crowded to obtain around the bus. (repeated words in italics) (46). H.M.: “Well this pie is- or the pie here was (is + Past) back here–” (brackets ours) 6.2. Final results H.M. created no extra minor word, morpheme, and phonological retrieval errors than the controls. The mean variety of word and morpheme retrieval errors per response was 0.00 for H.M. and 0.00 for the controls (SD = 0.00), with absolute Ns also tiny for meaningful statistical analysis. The only doable phonological retrieval error in the database was ambiguous: “Is it crowded” in (47) transposes either the phonological units s and t or the words is and it within the BPC It can be crowded. Even so, this error was neither a minor phonological error nor a minor word retrieval error simply because (a) it was uncorrected, and (b) it and is belong to diverse lexical categories (pronoun and copular verb). The mean number of minor phonological sequencing errors was for that reason 0.07 per response for H.M. versus 0.01 for the controls (SD = 0.04), a non-reliable 1.five SD distinction with Ns as well compact for meaningful analysis. (47). H.M.: “Is it crowded…” (BPC ba.